Receptive aphasia: Difference between revisions

Jump to navigation Jump to search
No edit summary
No edit summary
Line 14: Line 14:
}}
}}
{{SI}}
{{SI}}
{{CMG}}


{{EH}}
{{EH}}


==Overview==
==Overview==
'''Receptive aphasia''', also known as '''Wernicke’s aphasia,''' '''fluent aphasia,''' or '''sensory aphasia''' in [[clinical neuropsychology]] and [[cognitive neuropsychology]], is a type of [[aphasia]] often (but not always) caused by neurological damage to [[Wernicke's area|Wernicke’s area]] in the brain ([[Brodman area 22]], in the posterior part of the [[superior temporal gyrus]] of the dominant hemisphere). This is not to be confused with [[Wernicke's encephalopathy|Wernicke’s encephalopathy]] or [[Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome]].  The aphasia was first described by [[Carl Wernicke]] and its understanding substantially advanced by Norman Geschwind.
'''Receptive aphasia''', also known as '''Wernicke’s aphasia,''' '''fluent aphasia,''' or '''sensory aphasia''' in [[clinical neuropsychology]] and [[cognitive neuropsychology]], is a type of [[aphasia]] often (but not always) caused by neurological damage to [[Wernicke's area|Wernicke’s area]] in the brain ([[Brodman area 22]], in the posterior part of the [[superior temporal gyrus]] of the dominant hemisphere). This is not to be confused with [[Wernicke's encephalopathy|Wernicke’s encephalopathy]] or [[Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome]].  The aphasia was first described by [[Carl Wernicke]] and its understanding substantially advanced by Norman Geschwind.


Line 26: Line 28:


Example:
Example:
:I called my mother on the television and did not understand the door. It was too breakfast, but they came from far to near. My mother is not too old for me to be young.
:I called my mother on the television and did not understand the door. It was too breakfast, but they came from far to near. My mother is not too old for me to be young.



Revision as of 22:10, 18 January 2009

Template:DiseaseDisorder infobox

WikiDoc Resources for Receptive aphasia

Articles

Most recent articles on Receptive aphasia

Most cited articles on Receptive aphasia

Review articles on Receptive aphasia

Articles on Receptive aphasia in N Eng J Med, Lancet, BMJ

Media

Powerpoint slides on Receptive aphasia

Images of Receptive aphasia

Photos of Receptive aphasia

Podcasts & MP3s on Receptive aphasia

Videos on Receptive aphasia

Evidence Based Medicine

Cochrane Collaboration on Receptive aphasia

Bandolier on Receptive aphasia

TRIP on Receptive aphasia

Clinical Trials

Ongoing Trials on Receptive aphasia at Clinical Trials.gov

Trial results on Receptive aphasia

Clinical Trials on Receptive aphasia at Google

Guidelines / Policies / Govt

US National Guidelines Clearinghouse on Receptive aphasia

NICE Guidance on Receptive aphasia

NHS PRODIGY Guidance

FDA on Receptive aphasia

CDC on Receptive aphasia

Books

Books on Receptive aphasia

News

Receptive aphasia in the news

Be alerted to news on Receptive aphasia

News trends on Receptive aphasia

Commentary

Blogs on Receptive aphasia

Definitions

Definitions of Receptive aphasia

Patient Resources / Community

Patient resources on Receptive aphasia

Discussion groups on Receptive aphasia

Patient Handouts on Receptive aphasia

Directions to Hospitals Treating Receptive aphasia

Risk calculators and risk factors for Receptive aphasia

Healthcare Provider Resources

Symptoms of Receptive aphasia

Causes & Risk Factors for Receptive aphasia

Diagnostic studies for Receptive aphasia

Treatment of Receptive aphasia

Continuing Medical Education (CME)

CME Programs on Receptive aphasia

International

Receptive aphasia en Espanol

Receptive aphasia en Francais

Business

Receptive aphasia in the Marketplace

Patents on Receptive aphasia

Experimental / Informatics

List of terms related to Receptive aphasia

Editor-In-Chief: C. Michael Gibson, M.S., M.D. [1]

Please Take Over This Page and Apply to be Editor-In-Chief for this topic: There can be one or more than one Editor-In-Chief. You may also apply to be an Associate Editor-In-Chief of one of the subtopics below. Please mail us [2] to indicate your interest in serving either as an Editor-In-Chief of the entire topic or as an Associate Editor-In-Chief for a subtopic. Please be sure to attach your CV and or biographical sketch.

Overview

Receptive aphasia, also known as Wernicke’s aphasia, fluent aphasia, or sensory aphasia in clinical neuropsychology and cognitive neuropsychology, is a type of aphasia often (but not always) caused by neurological damage to Wernicke’s area in the brain (Brodman area 22, in the posterior part of the superior temporal gyrus of the dominant hemisphere). This is not to be confused with Wernicke’s encephalopathy or Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. The aphasia was first described by Carl Wernicke and its understanding substantially advanced by Norman Geschwind.

Presentation

If Wernicke’s area is damaged in the non-dominant hemisphere, the syndrome resulting will be sensory dysprosody - the lack of ability to perceive the pitch, rhythm, and emotional tone of speech.

Speech is preserved, but language content is incorrect. This may vary from the insertion of a few incorrect or nonexistent words to a profuse outpouring of jargon. Grammar, syntax, rate, intonation and stress are normal. Substitutions of one word for another (paraphasias, e.g. “telephone” for “television”) are common. Comprehension and repetition are poor.

Example:

I called my mother on the television and did not understand the door. It was too breakfast, but they came from far to near. My mother is not too old for me to be young.

If excessive, this may be confused with the psychiatric signs of “pressure of speech” and “word salad.”

Patients who recover from Wernicke’s aphasia report that, while aphasic, they found the speech of others to be unintelligible and, despite being cognizant of that fact that they were speaking, they could neither stop themselves nor understand their own words.

The ability to understand and repeat songs is usually unaffected, as these are processed by the opposite hemisphere. "Melodic intonation therapy" had been pursued for some years with aphasic patients under the belief that it helps stimulate the ability to speak normally, but in 2003 this was found to not be so.[1] Patients also generally have no trouble purposefully reciting anything they have memorized. The ability to utter profanity is also left unaffected, however the patient typically has no control over it, and may not even understand their own profanity.

Luria's theory on Wernicke's aphasia

Luria proposed that this type of aphasia has three characteristics.[2]

  • 1) A deficit in the categorization of sounds. In order to hear and understand what is said, one must be able to recognize the different sounds of spoken language. For example, hearing the difference between bad and bed is easy for native English speakers. The Dutch language however, makes no difference between these vowels, and therefore the Dutch have difficulties hearing the difference between them. This problem is exactly what patients with Wernicke’s aphasia have in their own language: they can't isolate significant sound characteristics and classify them into known meaningful systems.
  • 2) A defect in speech. A patient with Wernicke's aphasia can and may speak a great deal, but he or she confuses sound characteristics, producing “word salad”: intelligible words that appear to be strung together randomly.
  • 3) An impairment in writing. A person who cannot discern sounds cannot be expected to write.

See also

References

  1. Hébert, S. & Racette, A., Gagnon, L. & Peretz, I. (2003). Revisiting the dissociation between speaking and singing in aphasia. Brain, 126, 1838-1850. http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/reprint/126/8/1838
  2. Kolb & Whishaw: Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology (2003), pages 503-504. The whole paragraph on Luria's theory is written with help of this reference.

de:Wernicke-Aphasie


Template:WH Template:WS